69传媒

69传媒 & Literacy Explainer

How Do Kids Learn to Read? What the Science Says

By Sarah Schwartz & Sarah D. Sparks 鈥 October 02, 2019 | Corrected: October 04, 2019 23 min read
  • Save to favorites
  • Print
Email Copy URL

Corrected: A previous version of this article misstated the year Keith Stanovich coined the term 鈥淢atthew Effect in 69传媒.鈥 It was 1986.

NOTE: The text of this article contains annotations. To view the list of annotations, please scroll to the bottom of the page.

How do children learn to read?

For almost a century, researchers have argued over the question. Most of the disagreement has centered on the very beginning stages of the reading process, when young children are first starting to figure out how to decipher words on a page.

One theory is that reading is a natural process, like learning to speak. If teachers and parents surround children with good books, this theory goes, kids will pick up reading on their own. Another idea suggests that reading is a series of strategic guesses based on context, and that kids should be taught these guessing strategies.

But research has shown that reading is not a natural process(1), and it鈥檚 not a guessing game. Written language is a code. Certain combinations of letters predictably represent certain sounds. And for the last few decades, the research has been clear: Teaching young kids how to crack the code鈥攖eaching systematic phonics鈥攊s the most reliable way to make sure that they learn how to read words.

Of course, there is more to reading than seeing a word on a page and pronouncing it out loud. As such, there is more to teaching reading than just teaching phonics. 69传媒 requires children to make meaning out of print. They need to know the different sounds in spoken language and be able to connect those sounds to written letters in order to decipher words. They need deep background and vocabulary knowledge so that they understand the words they read. Eventually, they need to be able to recognize most words automatically and read connected text fluently, attending to grammar, punctuation, and sentence structure.

See Also

reading main landing image social
Sevans/Education Week

But knowing how to decode is an essential step in becoming a reader. If children can鈥檛 decipher the precise words on the page, they鈥檒l never become fluent readers or understand the passages they鈥檙e reading.

That鈥檚 why we鈥檝e put together this overview of the research on early reading, in grades K-2. It covers what鈥檚 known about how we should teach letter-sound patterns, and what we don鈥檛 know for sure yet. It touches on what else should be part of early reading programs. And it explains why we know that most children can鈥檛 learn to read through osmosis or guessing.

Here鈥檚 what the evidence shows.

Don鈥檛 children learn to read the way they learn to speak?

Infants learn to speak (2) by listening to and repeating sounds made by adults and connecting them to meanings. They don鈥檛 consciously distinguish individual sound units (called phonemes) when hearing spoken language. Some research suggests infants learn probabilistically鈥攆or example, hearing the sound 鈥渂all鈥 at the same time as the sight of a round, bouncy object over time makes the child associate the two鈥攚hile other studies suggest children map meaning to a word after experiencing it just once or twice. Within the first two years, typically developing toddlers鈥 brains focus on the most common sounds in their native languages and connect those sounds to meaning. A child develops understanding of speech through exposure to language and opportunities to practice the 鈥渟erve and return鈥(3) patterns of conversation, even without explicit instruction.

By contrast, children do not naturally develop reading skill through exposure to text. The way they learn to connect oral and written language(4) depends on what kind of language(5) they are learning to read.

Alphabetic languages, like English or French, use letters to stand for sounds that make up spoken words. To read an alphabetic language, children must learn how written letters represent spoken sounds(6), recognize patterns of letter sounds as words, and match those to spoken words whose meanings they know. This differs from Chinese, for example. It uses a tonal spoken language, conveying meaning with small differences in stress or pitch. Its writing system is partially logographic鈥攊n which written symbols correspond directly to a word or concept鈥攁nd also includes words that couple symbols for meaning and symbols for sound. Someone reading Chinese hanzi characters could not 鈥渟ound out鈥 unfamiliar words character by character(7).

What is systematic, explicit phonics instruction, and why is it important?

Connecting printed letters on a page to written sounds isn鈥檛 intuitive(8). While some young children may make those connections themselves, most do not. One set of studies from 1989-90 illustrates this phenomenon well(9).

In these studies, conducted by Brian Byrne and Ruth Fielding-Barnsley, researchers taught young children between ages 3 and 5 to read whole words aloud, like 鈥渇at鈥 and 鈥渂at.鈥 These children didn鈥檛 already know their letter names.

Then, the researchers tested whether the children could transfer their knowledge to reading a new word. They gave them the word 鈥渇un,鈥 and asked whether the word was 鈥渇un鈥 or 鈥渂un.鈥 Very few of the students could do this successfully. They couldn鈥檛 break down the original word into phonemes and then transfer their knowledge of those phonemes to a new word.

But children could succeed on this task if they were first given some explicit instructions. When children were taught how to recognize that certain letters represented certain sounds, and taught how to segment words to identify those individual letters and sounds, they had much greater success on the original transfer test. Neuroscience research has since confirmed and helped explain these findings. When learning how to read new words in an unfamiliar made-up language, participants had more long-term success if they were first taught which symbols correspond to which sounds, than if they tried to remember words as wholes. Brain imaging of these readers finds that the two teaching strategies tap into different neural pathways in the brain. Readers taught to connect print to meaning directly could recall words initially more quickly, but less accurately; readers taught to connect print to sound and then to meaning read aloud more quickly and correctly, better recalled the correct meanings of words, and transferred their knowledge to new words.

Decades of research has shown that explicit phonics instruction benefits early readers, but particularly those who struggle to read.

That鈥檚 because small strengths or deficits at the start of reading compound over time. It鈥檚 what reading expert Keith Stanovich in 1986 dubbed(10) the 鈥淢atthew Effect in 69传媒,鈥(11) after the Bible verse in which the rich get richer and the poor get poorer: 鈥淭he combination of deficient decoding skills, lack of practice, and difficult materials results in unrewarding early reading experiences that lead to less involvement in reading-related activities,鈥 Stanovich wrote. 鈥淟ack of exposure and practice on the part of the less-skilled reader delays the development of automaticity and speed at the word recognition level. Slow, capacity-draining word-recognition processes require cognitive resources that should be allocated to comprehension. Thus, reading for meaning is hindered; unrewarding reading experiences multiply; and practice is avoided or merely tolerated without real cognitive involvement.鈥

My reading curriculum includes letter-sound instruction. Am I providing enough phonics?

Not all phonics instruction is created equal.

The most effective phonics programs are those that are systematic. The National 69传媒 Panel found this in 2000(12), and since then, further research reviews have confirmed that this type of instruction leads to the greatest gains in reading accuracy for young students(13).

A systematic phonics program teaches an ordered progression of letter-sound correspondences. Teachers don鈥檛 only address the letter-sound connections that students stumble over. Instead, they address all of the combinations methodically, in a sequence, moving on to the next once students demonstrate mastery. Teachers explicitly tell students what sounds correspond to what letter patterns, rather than asking students to figure it out on their own or make guesses.

In one series of experiments(14), Stanford University neuroscientist Bruce McCandliss(15) and his colleagues made up a new written language and taught three-letter words to students either by asking them to focus on letter sounds or on whole words. Later, the students took a reading test of both the words they were taught and new words in the made-up language, while an electroencephalograph monitored their brain activity. Those who had focused on letter sounds had more neural activity on the left side of the brain, which includes visual and language regions and is associated with more skilled reading. Those who had been taught to focus on whole words had more activity on the right side of the brain, which has been characteristically associated with adults and children who struggle with reading. Moreover, those who had learned letter sounds were better able to identify unfamiliar words.

Early readers benefit from systematic phonics instruction. Among students in grades K-1, phonics instruction led to improvements in decoding ability and reading comprehension across the board, according to the National 69传媒 Panel(16). Children at risk of developing future reading problems, children with disabilities, and children from all socio-economic backgrounds all benefited. Later research reviews have confirmed that systematic phonics instruction is effective for students with disabilities, and shown that it also works for English-language learners(17).

Most studies of phonics instruction test its immediate effectiveness鈥攁fter the intervention, are children better readers? Among students in older grades, the results are less clear. A recent meta-analysis of the long-term effects of reading interventions(18) looked at phonics and phonemic awareness training, mostly in studies with children in grades K-1. Both phonics and phonemic awareness interventions improved reading comprehension at an immediate post-test. But while the benefits of phonemic awareness interventions persisted in a follow-up test, the benefits of phonics interventions faded much more over time. The average length of all interventions included in the study was about 40 hours, and the follow-up assessments were conducted about a year after the interventions were complete, on average.

Some of my students didn鈥檛 need phonics instruction to learn to read. Why are you saying that all kids benefit?

Depending on the estimate(19), anywhere from 1 percent to 7 percent of children figure out how to decode words on their own, without explicit instruction. They may spot the patterns in books read to them or print they see in their environment, and then they apply these patterns. These include children with a neurotypical form of 鈥渉yperlexia鈥(20)鈥攁 condition in which children may begin decoding as early as 3鈥攂ut this is more frequently associated with children who have autism-spectrum disorders and often have separate problems with reading comprehension.

It may seem like these children are reading words as whole units, or using guessing strategies to figure out what comes next in the story. But they are attending to all of the words鈥 individual letters鈥攖hey鈥檙e just doing it very quickly(21).

A systematic phonics program can still benefit these students, who may have gaps in their knowledge of spelling patterns or words that they haven鈥檛 encountered yet. Of course, phonics instruction鈥攍ike all teaching鈥攃an and should be differentiated to meet the needs of individual students where they are. If a student can demonstrate mastery of a sound, there鈥檚 no need to continue practicing that sound鈥攈e or she should move on to the next one.

There鈥檚 another answer to this question: 69传媒 may look like they鈥檙e decoding when they鈥檙e actually not. For example, a child may see an illustration of an apple falling from a tree, and correctly guess that the sentence below the picture describes an apple falling from a tree. This isn鈥檛 reading, and it doesn鈥檛 give the teacher useful information about how a student will tackle a book without pictures.

Can cueing strategies help students to read?

Many early reading classrooms teach students strategies to identify a word by guessing with the help of context cues. Ken and Yetta Goodman of the University of Arizona(22) developed a 鈥渢hree-cueing system,鈥 based on analysis of common errors (or 鈥渕iscues鈥) when students read aloud. Ken Goodman famously called reading development a 鈥減sycholinguistic guessing game,鈥(23) and cueing systems teach students to guess at a new word based on:

  • Meaning/Semantics, or background knowledge and context, such as vocabulary a student has already learned;
  • Structure/Syntax, or how the word fits in common grammar rules, such as whether the word鈥檚 position in a sentence suggests it is a noun, verb, or adjective; and
  • Visual/Graphophonics, or what a word looks like, such as how upper- and lowercase letters are used (suggesting a proper noun, for example) or common spelling patterns.

Cueing systems are a common strategy in whole-language programs, and also are used in many 鈥渂alanced literacy鈥 programs that incorporate phonics instruction. Cueing systems were designed by analyzing errors(24) rather than practices of proficient readers, and have not shown benefits in controlled experiments(25).

Moreover, cognitive and neuroscience studies have found that guessing is a much less efficient way to identify a new word, and a mark of beginning or struggling readers, not proficient readers. Skilled readers instead sound out new words to decode them.

Balanced literacy programs often include both phonics and cueing, but studies suggest cueing instruction can make it more difficult for children to develop phonics skills because it takes their attention away from the letter sounds.

I know phonics instruction is supposed to be explicit and systematic. But beyond that, how should I teach it? Does the research say anything about what content I need to cover, and how should it be sequenced?

There is a general path that most children follow as they become skilled decoders. Research can tell us how children usually progress along this path, and which skills specifically predict better reading performance.

Before starting kindergarten, children generally develop some early phonological awareness鈥攁n understanding of the sounds that make up spoken language. They can rhyme, break down multi-syllable words, and recognize alliteration(26).

A next step in the process is understanding that graphemes鈥攃ombinations of one or more letters鈥攔epresent phonemes, the smallest units of spoken language. It鈥檚 easier for students to learn these letter-sound correspondences(27) if they already have early phonological skills like rhyming and alliteration, along with knowledge of the names of the letters of the alphabet.

And while vocabulary is important for reading comprehension, research has also found that it鈥檚 a component in decoding ability. One study found that when children know a word鈥檚 meaning, they can more quickly learn how to recognize it automatically(28), because the visual letters, corresponding sounds, and meaning all map together when a reader recognizes a word.

There are other early skills that relate to later reading and writing ability as well(29), regardless of IQ or socio-economic status. Among these are writing letters, remembering spoken information for a short time, rapidly naming sequences of random letters, numbers, or pictures, and other phonological skills鈥攍ike the ability to segment words into phonemes.

To decode words, students need to be taught to blend together the phonemes that graphemes represent on the page. For example, a young reader must learn to recognize that /r/, /o/, /d/ are three sounds that together form the word 鈥渞od,鈥 but also that the word 鈥渞ock鈥 also contains three sounds, /r/, /o/, /k/ This is a process that builds on itself rapidly. Though there are some 15,000 syllables in English(30), after a child has learned the 44 most common sound and letter combinations(31), they will begin to sound out words as they read. These include both the basic letter and vowel sounds, but also common combinations such as 鈥渢h,鈥 鈥渟h,鈥 and 鈥-ing.鈥 There are two main ways to demonstrate to children that words are made up of sound-letter correspondences. In one method, students learn the sounds of the letters first and then blend these phonemes together to sound out words. That鈥檚 synthetic phonics鈥攖hey鈥檙e synthesizing phonemes into greater whole words. The other method, analytic phonics, takes an inverted approach: 69传媒 identify鈥攐r analyze鈥攖he phonemes within words, and then use that knowledge to read other words.

Take the word 鈥渂at.鈥 In synthetic phonics, students would first learn the /b/ sound, then the /a/ sound, then the /t/ sound and blend them together to sound out 鈥渂at.鈥 In analytic phonics, students would learn the word 鈥渂at鈥 alongside words like 鈥渃at,鈥 鈥渕at,鈥 and 鈥渉at,鈥 and would be taught that all these words end in the 鈥渁t鈥 sound pattern.

So there鈥檚 synthetic phonics and analytic phonics鈥攊s one way better than the other?

A few studies have found synthetic phonics to be more effective than analytic phonics. Most notably, a seven-year longitudinal study from Scotland found that synthetic phonics taught in 1st grade gave students an advantage in reading and spelling over analytic phonics(32). Still, when examined as a whole, the larger body of reading research doesn鈥檛 surface a conclusive winner. Two landmark research reviews(33) haven鈥檛 found a significant difference in the effectiveness of the two methods. Other more recent research is still inconclusive(34).

Do these strategies apply to words that don鈥檛 follow traditional sound-spelling patterns? What about words like 鈥渙ne鈥 and 鈥渇riend鈥濃攃an those words still be taught with phonics?

Yes, but not alone; spelling and semantic rules go hand-in-hand with teaching letter sounds. Words like 鈥渓ime鈥 and 鈥渄ime,鈥 have similar spelling and pronunciation. But some words with similar spelling have different pronunciations, like 鈥減int鈥 and 鈥渕int.鈥 And others have different spellings and similar pronunciations, like 鈥渏azz鈥 and 鈥渉as.鈥 Brain imaging studies(35) find that when readers see word pairs that are inconsistent, they show greater activity in the areas of the brain associated with processing both visual spelling and spoken words. This shows that young readers use systems of understanding of both printed shapes and sounds when they see any written word. When those two systems conflict, the reader may call on additional rules, such as understanding that words at the end of lines of a rhyming poem (such as 鈥渉as鈥 and 鈥渏azz鈥) likely rhyme even if their spelling would not suggest it.

Some research has found that teaching common irregular words, like 鈥渙ne鈥 and 鈥渇riend,鈥 as sight words can be effective(36). Still, in these studies, children were also taught phonics along with sight words鈥攁nd that鈥檚 important. Understanding phonics gives students the foundation to read these irregular words. Take 鈥渇riend.鈥 While the 鈥渋e鈥 doesn鈥檛 produce the same sound it normally does, the other letters in the word do. Research has suggested(37) that children use the 鈥渇r鈥 and the 鈥渘d鈥 as a framework when they remember how to read the irregular word 鈥渇riend.鈥

When should children start to learn how to sound out words? Is there a 鈥渢oo early鈥?

Even very young children can benefit from instruction designed to develop phonological awareness. The National Early Literacy Panel Report (2009)(38), a meta-analysis of early literacy studies, found that teaching preschoolers and kindergartners how to distinguish the sounds in words, whether orally or in relationship to print, improved their reading and writing ability. The children in these studies were generally between the ages of 3 and 5.

Studies suggest progress in phonics is less closely linked to a child鈥檚 age than to the size and complexity of his spoken vocabulary, and to his opportunities to practice and apply new phonics rules. There is some evidence(39) that 鈥渄ecodable鈥 books(40), designed to help students practice specific letter-sound combinations, can benefit the earliest readers. But it is mixed, and students very quickly progress enough to get more benefit from texts that provide more complex and irregular words鈥攁nd often texts that students find more interesting.

How much time should teachers spend on teaching about letters and sounds in class?

There isn鈥檛 yet a definitive 鈥渂est鈥 amount of time to spend on phonics instruction. In several meta-analyses, researchers haven鈥檛 found a direct link between program length and effectiveness(41).

The National 69传媒 Panel report found that programs focusing on phonemic awareness, the ability to hear, identify, and manipulate the smallest units of speech sounds, that lasted less than 20 hours total had the greatest effect on reading skills. Across the studies that the researchers looked at, individual sessions lasted 25 minutes on average.

But the authors of the NRP are quick to point out that these patterns are descriptive, not prescriptive. The studies they looked at weren鈥檛 specifically testing the effectiveness of different time lengths, and it may be that time wasn鈥檛 the relevant factor in these shorter programs performing better.

Eventually, a skilled reader doesn鈥檛 need to sound out every word that she reads. She sees the word and recognizes it immediately. Through reading the word again and again over time, her brain has linked this particular sequence to this word, through a process called orthographic mapping(42).

But neuroscience research has shown that even if it feels like she鈥檚 recognizing the word as a whole, she鈥檚 still attending to the sequence of individual letters in the word for an incredibly short period of time. That鈥檚 how skilled readers can tell the difference between the words 鈥渁ccent鈥 and 鈥渁scent.鈥

What else鈥攁side from phonics鈥攊s part of a research-based early reading program?

Phonics is essential to a research-based reading program. If students can鈥檛 decode words, they can鈥檛 derive any meaning from them. But understanding the alphabetic code doesn鈥檛 automatically make students good readers. There are five essential components of reading(43): phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension.

The National 69传媒 Panel addressed all five of these components. The researchers found that having students read out loud with guidance and feedback improved reading fluency. Vocabulary instruction, both explicit and implicit, led to better reading comprehension鈥攁nd it was most effective when students had multiple opportunities to see and use new words in context. They also found that teaching comprehension strategies can also lead to gains in reading achievement, though most of these studies were done with students older than 2nd grade.

For younger students, oral language skills; understanding syntax, grammar, vocabulary, and idioms; and having general and topic-specific background knowledge are also essential for reading comprehension.

This is one of the premises of the Simple View of 69传媒(44), a framework to understand reading first proposed by researchers Philip B. Gough and William E. Tunmer in 1986(45). In the simple view, reading comprehension is the product of decoding ability and language comprehension. If a student can鈥檛 decode, it doesn鈥檛 matter how much background knowledge and vocabulary(46) he understands鈥攈e won鈥檛 be able to understand what鈥檚 on the page. But the opposite is also true: If a student can decode but doesn鈥檛 have a deep enough understanding of oral language, he won鈥檛 be able to understand the words he can say out loud. Since Gough and Tunmer first proposed this framework, many studies have confirmed its basic structure鈥攖hat comprehension and decoding are separate processes(47). One meta-analysis of reading intervention studies finds that phonics-focused interventions were most effective through grade 1; in older grades鈥攚hen most students will have mastered phonics鈥攊nterventions that targeted comprehension or a mix of reading skills showed bigger effects(48) on students鈥 reading skills.

For young students, early oral-language interventions can help set them up for success even before they start formal school.

The National Early Literacy Panel found that both reading books to young children and engaging in activities aimed at improving their language development improved their oral language skills(49).

If children don鈥檛 learn to read naturally from being exposed to reading, why are parents and teachers encouraged to read to infants and preschoolers?

The amount of time adults read with preschoolers and young children(50) does predict their reading skills in elementary school. One of the most important predictors of how well a child will learn to read is the size and quality of his spoken language and vocabulary, and children are more likely to be exposed to new words and their meanings or pick up grammar rules from reading aloud with adults.

In a series of studies in the late 1990s of 5-year-olds who had not yet learned to read, Victoria Purcell-Gates(51) found that after controlling for the income and education level of the children鈥檚 parents, children who had been read to regularly in the last two years used more 鈥渓iterary鈥 language, longer phrases, and more sophisticated sentence structures. Moreover, an adult reading with a child is more likely to explain or expand on the meanings of words and concepts that the child does not already know, adding to their background knowledge.

69传媒 with trusted adults also helps children develop a love of reading. 鈥淭he association between hearing written language and feeling loved provides the best foundation for this long process [of emergent literacy], and no cognitive scientist or educational researcher could have designed a better one,鈥 notes cognitive neuroscientist Maryanne Wolf(52).

What about independent choice reading?

In a choice reading period鈥攁lso known as sustained silent reading or Drop Everything and Read鈥攕tudents get to pick a book to read independently in class for a set amount of time. The premise behind this activity is that children need time to practice reading skills on their own to improve.

There is a lot of correlational research that shows that children who read more are better readers(53). But many of these studies don鈥檛 quantify how much reading students are actually doing. While they may specify a time frame鈥15 minutes of sustained silent reading, for example鈥攖he studies don鈥檛 report whether kids spend this time reading. That makes it difficult to know how effective choice reading actually is.

More importantly, these studies don鈥檛 provide experimental evidence鈥攊t鈥檚 not clear whether reading more is what makes students better readers, or if better readers are likely to read more. The National 69传媒 Panel found that there wasn鈥檛 evidence that choice reading improved students鈥 fluency.

Does it make a difference whether children learn to read using printed books or digital ones?

In the last decade or so, access to Internet-based text has continued to expand, and schools have increasingly used digitally based books(54), particularly to support students who do not have easy access to paper books at home. Yet some emerging evidence suggests children learn to read differently in print versus digitally(55), in ways that could hinder their later comprehension.

Researchers that study eye movements find that those reading digital text are more likely to skim or read nonlinearly, looking for key words to give the gist, jump to the end to find conclusions or takeaways, and only sometimes go back to find context in the rest of the text. In a separate series of studies since 2015(56), researchers led by Anne Mangen found that students who read short stories and especially longer texts in a print format were better able to remember the plot and sequence of events than those who read the same text on a screen.

It鈥檚 not yet clear how universal these changes are, but teachers may want to keep watch on how well their students reading electronically are developing deeper reading and comprehension skills.

Related Education Week Stories

1. For one early argument, see 鈥溾.

2. For more, read 鈥溾.

3. 鈥Key to Vocabulary Gap Is Quality of Conversation, Not Dearth of Words

4. In this video, cognitive psychologist Daniel Willingham discusses the implications of the science of reading with Education Week assistant managing editor Liana Heitin Loewus. For Willingham鈥檚 book, .

5. 鈥溾

6. One way to see the connection between sound recognition and reading is to look at the effects of one of the most common early childhood illnesses, middle-ear infections. Studies have found children who had repeated infections before age 3 had lower reading performance in grades 1 and 2 than children without ear infections. The infections were directly related to lower phonological awareness, alliteration, and rhyme, as well as expressive language, word reading and definitions. 鈥溾

7. Scholars of historic language development suggest that one of the earliest writing systems, Sumerian cuneiform, evolved from a mostly logographic system to one that used characters to represent sounds in its spoken language. See: . You can also see Wolf discuss the evolution of spoken versus written language .

8. Hear teachers鈥 perspectives on phonics instruction .

9. For the full studies, see: 鈥溾 and 鈥溾.

10. Keith Stanovich discusses how early differences build over time .

11. Stanovich adapted the concept from sociologists Robert Merton and Harriet Zuckerman. The 鈥溾 has been applied to areas from law to geopolitics as a way to describe how early advantage builds on itself.

12. Overall, systematic phonics programs produced a moderate and statistically significant effect size, d=0.44. The report concluded that these findings suggest systematic phonics programs have a greater positive effect than other programs that provide unsystematic phonics, or no phonics. See 鈥.鈥

13. This 2006 review found that systematic phonics instruction had a moderate and statistically significant effect on reading accuracy. The review didn鈥檛 find that phonics had a statistically significant effect on reading comprehension, but the authors note that this finding was based on weak evidence鈥搊nly four randomized controlled trials that examined comprehension were included. 鈥溾.
This determined systematic phonics instruction to be an essential component of reading instruction, as part of a program that explicitly teaches phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension.

14. 鈥.鈥 For a broader discussion of the experiments, see:

15. In a Stanford University lecture, .

16. 鈥溾

17.
The review found that phonics instruction had a small but statistically significant effect on reading performance across the studies in children and adolescents. Studies in children with mild reading disabilities had a greater effect than studies in children with moderate and severe reading disabilities.

English-language learners need the well-rounded reading instruction that their native English speaking peers do鈥搃nstruction that integrates phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary, fluency, and comprehension. Still, within phonemic awareness, it may be most effective to give students more work with phonemes that appear in English, but not in their native language. Oral language development in English is also especially important for ELLs.

18. .

19. 鈥,鈥 鈥,鈥 鈥溾

20. 鈥.鈥

21. Research has shown that when young readers know words on sight, it鈥檚 because they recognize the sound-letter patterns in those words鈥搉ot because they鈥檝e stored the words in their visual memory. See: 鈥,鈥

22.

23. 鈥溾.

24. 鈥溾

25. This is laid out in several reports, including 鈥溾 鈥,鈥 and 鈥.鈥

26. 鈥溾 (book).

27. 鈥.鈥
鈥溾
鈥.鈥

28.

29. 鈥溾

30. Psycholinguistics expert Mark Seidenberg estimates the English language has as many as 15,000 syllables. See his book: 鈥溾

31. You can hear pronunciations of the most common English phonemes .

32. For the Scotland study, see 鈥.鈥
The British government also commissioned a review of the research in teaching early reading, published in 2006, that suggested synthetic phonics was more effective. This recommendation was made based on observations of classroom practice, though, not experimental research. For this review, see 鈥.鈥

33. Both the National 69传媒 Panel and a 2006 reading review didn鈥檛 find a significant difference between the two methods.
鈥,鈥
鈥溾

34. 鈥溾
This 2017 review of recent research comparing analytic and synthetic phonics found that both methods were effective for teaching young and struggling readers.

35. 鈥溾

36. 鈥溾
In this study, two groups of early elementary school students were taught with different programs: One was taught multiple letter-sound correspondences, and the other was taught the most consistent letter-sound patterns along with sight words. Overall, the two programs were equally effective, and for both groups of students, phonological awareness was significantly related to reading ability. But within the program that only taught letter-sound patterns, there was a closer link between phonological awareness and ability to read words that broke traditional phonemic rules. On the other hand, children who had less phonological awareness when they started school improved more in reading under the program that also taught sight words.

37. 鈥溾 In this 2005 paper, reading researcher Linnea Ehri theorized that this is how children remember irregular words. Research studies, conducted both before and after this paper was released, lend support to this framework.
For example, see 鈥.鈥
In these experiments, subjects were more likely to judge incorrectly spelled words as correct if they had been repeatedly exposed to the incorrect spellings. Other research has shown that young children who have not yet developed phonemic awareness can use letter names to make sense of words.
For example, see 鈥,鈥 and 鈥溾

38. 鈥溾

39. 鈥.鈥 For a discussion of research on decodable books, see:
鈥溾

40. For an interesting discussion of the pros and cons of decodable text, see:
鈥溾

41. 鈥溾, 鈥溾, 鈥溾

42. Orthographic mapping is why we can recognize words on sight. In this process, a reader anchors the written sequence of letters that make up a word to the pronunciation and definition that she knows for that word. Eventually, she doesn鈥檛 need to actively decode the word anymore鈥搒he automatically recognizes the letter-sound patterns and knows that they represent the word in her vocabulary. This happens over time, as the reader is exposed repeatedly to words that he or she can decode and comprehend. To do this, readers need a well-developed oral vocabulary and strong phonemic awareness skills. Empirical evidence for this process was first found by Linnea Ehri, an educational psychologist at Graduate Center of the City University of New York. See: 鈥溾 For a review of the research, see:
鈥溾

43. Later, the National Early Literacy Panel added to this, noting six skills that predicted later reading skills even after accounting for a child鈥檚 IQ or other background information:

  • alphabet knowledge, or understanding names and sounds associated with specific written letters;
  • phonological awareness, the ability to understand auditory aspects of speech independent of its meaning, including distinguishing individual syllables or phonemes;
  • rapid automatic naming (RAN) of a random series of numbers or letters;
  • RAN of a series of pictures of colors or objects;
  • Naming, the ability to write individual spoken letters or to write one鈥檚 name; and
  • Phonological memory, or the ability to recall spoken information after a brief period of time.

鈥溾

44. 鈥溾

45. 鈥 SAGE Journals

46.

47. 鈥"; 鈥"; 鈥溾. Other research suggests that there are additional factors that influence students鈥 reading comprehension abilities, such as their ability to pay attention to specific tasks. See:

48.

49.

50. Based on a meta-analysis of interventions the National Early Literacy Panel found shared reading significantly improved young children鈥檚 spoken language skills and print knowledge. To put the effects in context, the panel noted that if average children who were not read to would have scored a 100 on a test of oral language, the children who were read to would have performed 11 points higher. 鈥溾

51. 鈥淧arents as Literacy Brokers鈥 in 鈥淭he Routledge International Handbook of Early Literacy Education鈥

52.

53. 鈥溾
- John T. Guthrie, 2004
For a review, see 鈥溾 Some other correlational research has suggested that guided independent reading, with some intervention or direction from the teacher, has a positive effect on comprehension. See 鈥溾

54. The effects of digital storybooks seem to depend on specific aspects. One analysis of 43 studies found that multimedia elements such as animated pictures or sound effects improved comprehension. Yet interactive elements, such as built-in games or pop-ups for dictionaries or additional information, distracted students and hurt comprehension. 鈥溾

55.

56. 鈥溾
鈥溾
鈥溾

Related Tags:

Further 69传媒









How to Cite This Article
Schwartz, Sarah and Sparks, Sarah D. (2019, October 2). How Do Kids Learn to Read? What the Science Says. Education Week. Retrieved Month Day, Year from /teaching-learning/how-do-kids-learn-to-read-what-the-science-says/2019/10

An alternative version of this article was published in the December 4, 2019 edition of Education Week.
A version of this article appeared in the December 04, 2019 edition of Education Week as How Do Kids Learn to Read?

Events

This content is provided by our sponsor. It is not written by and does not necessarily reflect the views of Education Week's editorial staff.
Sponsor
Artificial Intelligence Webinar
AI and Educational Leadership: Driving Innovation and Equity
Discover how to leverage AI to transform teaching, leadership, and administration. Network with experts and learn practical strategies.
Content provided by 
This content is provided by our sponsor. It is not written by and does not necessarily reflect the views of Education Week's editorial staff.
Sponsor
School Climate & Safety Webinar
Investing in Success: Leading a Culture of Safety and Support
Content provided by 
Assessment K-12 Essentials Forum Making Competency-Based Learning a Reality
Join this free virtual event to hear from educators and experts working to implement competency-based education.

EdWeek Top School Jobs

Teacher Jobs
Search over ten thousand teaching jobs nationwide 鈥 elementary, middle, high school and more.
Principal Jobs
Find hundreds of jobs for principals, assistant principals, and other school leadership roles.
Administrator Jobs
Over a thousand district-level jobs: superintendents, directors, more.
Support Staff Jobs
Search thousands of jobs, from paraprofessionals to counselors and more.

Read Next

69传媒 & Literacy Quiz Quiz Yourself: How Much Do You Know About Student Literacy Data?
Answer 7 questions about the importance of student literacy data and how to collect and use it.
69传媒 & Literacy Q&A What Is Disciplinary Literacy?
Tim Shanahan's research helped crystallize the idea of "discipline specific literacy." How has it evolved?
10 min read
Illustration of directional signs and book.
Dan Page for Education Week
69传媒 & Literacy What Happens When Every Teacher in a School Has the Tools to Improve 69传媒?
In a whole-school literacy initiative, students learn metacognitive tools to help with reading and then apply them across content areas.
8 min read
Illustration of words being highlighted.
Dan Page for Education Week
69传媒 & Literacy Do Leveled Books Have Any Place in the Classroom?
As the "science of reading" movement has spread, predictable texts for beginning readers have come under fire.
6 min read
Illustration of stacked books and ladder.
Getty Images Plus